The Genetics of Violence
Introduction
We, in the 1990?s, are slowly and inevitably being faced with the
sociological and biological implications of impending genetic power. This power
is analytical, in such cases as the Human Genome Project, which will hopefully
succeed in mapping out the genetic code for the entire human genetic composition.
Moreover, this power is preventative and participatory in that it can be, and is
being, used to control the behavior of humans and other animals. This new power,
in the eyes of many, is as risky and potentially hazardous as atomic energy: it
must be treated carefully, used under close supervision, performed under
professional consent and observation, otherwise, people will begin to see this
new genetic power as a dangerous drawback, rather than an advancement of human
culture.
One of the most highly contested and objectionable topics of genetic
power is the analysis of crime, violence, and impulsivity. Doubtless, most will
agree that children are not born with a natural affinity for violence and crime;
yet, new genetic studies are beginning down a long road of finding the
hereditary basis for impulsivity. While these studies continue to search for the
genetic source of aggression, child testing programs, drug manufacturers, civil
rights activists, lawyers, and anxious citizens await the resulting testimony of
the scientists. The social implications of the genetic search for aggressive
tendency is seen by some as a great step forward, by others as a dangerous power
with the ability to give birth to another Holocaust, and by still others as
racist.
At one time, it was believed that one?s character could be determined
from the bumps in one?s skull. Much later, in the 1960?s, as science marched on
in its regular pace, it was theorized that carriers of an extra Y (male)
chromosome were predisposed to criminality. Today, we are faced with the power
to determine and alter one?s character through genetics. We must collectively
decide whether the ultimate price, not of money but of natural evolution, is
worth the ultimate result.
Behavioral Genetics and Aggression
One day in 1978 a woman entered the University Hospital of Nijmegen, the
Netherlands, with complaints regarding the men in her family. Many of the men
seemed to have some sort of mental debility, including her brothers and her son.
In time, a pattern of strange behavior of the men emerged: one had raped his
sister, and, upon being institutionalized, stabbed a warden in the chest with a
pitchfork; another tried to run over his boss in an automobile after he had
criticized the man?s work; a third had a regular habit of making his sisters
undress at knife point, and two more were convicted arsonists. Additionally, the
known IQ?s of the men were typically around 85. The history of this sort of
behavior was found to be typical, as nine other males in the family, tracing
back to 1870, had the same type of disorder. It became evident that there was
something wrong in the lineage of the family. Hans Brunner, a geneticist at the
University Hospital, has been studying the family since 1988.
It was discovered that the men had a defect on the X chromosome that
helps regulate aggressive behavior. Brunner was cued to the fact that the defect
was on the X chromosome because the trait was passed on from mother to son, and
none of the women, with two X chromosomes, were afflicted. The gene normally
codes for the production of the enzyme monoamine oxidase A (MAOA), which breaks
down three important neurotransmitters that trigger or inhibit the transmission
of nerve impulses. One of these neurotransmitters is norepinephrine, which
raises blood pressure and increases alertness as part of the body?s “fight or
flight” mechanism. Brunner believes that the lack of this neurotransmitter
could cause an excess of chemical messages to the brain, in times of stress,
causing the victim?s fury. The men?s urine found extremely low levels of the
breakdown products of the three neurotransmitters, which are the breakdown
products after MAOA has done its work.
Another of the chemicals is serotonin, which inhibits the effects of
spontaneous neuronal firing, and consequently exerts a calming effect. The lack
of this inhibitor is held responsible for the “Jekyll and Hyde” personalities of
the afflicted men, and may be responsible for their low IQ?s.
Over the course of four years, Brunner was the first to ever link and
pinpoint a single gene to aggression. Also, he analyzed the X chromosomes of 28
members of the family, compiling sufficient evidence to prove his discovery.
However, Brunner never studied the influence of a shared environment on the men.
Many other factors of genetic and biochemical signals have been shown to
greatly influence behavior. In humans, impulsive aggression has been linked to
low concentrations of a chemical known as 5-HIAA in the cerebrospinal fluid.
Scientists have found a human gene which lies on chromosome 6 that creates a 25
percent higher susceptibility to schizophrenia. Also, MAOA has been found
responsible for REM sleep deprivation in rats, which increases the incidence of
fighting among the animals. Testosterone levels in repeated sex offenders is,
almost without exception, extremely high. The National Research Council (NRC)
reports that female mice and rhesus monkeys which have been injected with
testosterone, in utero or at birth, repeatedly show more aggression at adulthood
than others of their kind. Girls exposed to androgenic steroids in utero have
an increased tendency to be more aggressive than their piers, where boys
injected with anti-androgenic drugs were not as aggressive as their peers. The
neurotransmitter gamma-aminobutyric acid has been shown not only to inhibit
aggression, but may stimulate the brain as well. This may be the reason that
the IQ?s of the afflicted Dutch men were so low. In any case, all of these
chemicals, in a natural setting, are ultimately determined by the genetic
composition of the individual, and ample evidence exists that instances of
aggressive behavior and crime are closely related to genetics.
However, the relation between the environment, genetics, and aggression
has not yet been combined. Psychology and behavioral genetics, unfortunately,
are not combined as they sensibly should be. We know that Brunner never studied
the effects of the environment on the Dutch men; yet, experimentation with
animals has shown that, for example, aggressively bred mice can act non-
aggressively if placed in the right social environment. Therefore, the name of
“behavioral genetics” is finally beginning to live up to the literal meaning of
its name through the study of social and environmental influences.
Parental Aggression and Genetics
While there is very little known about the combined effects of genetics
and the environment, there is much to be said about the social tendency toward
violence with regard to the genetics of offspring. For example, parents are 60
to 70 times more likely to kill their children under the age of two if they are
not their genetic children. Fewer children are murdered by their stepparents as
the age of the children increases, but, nonetheless, a much higher number of
stepchildren are killed than genetic children. Moreover, male animals in the
wild, such as mice and monkeys, often kill the offspring of their mate if the
offspring is the product of another liaison. In humans, tribal men in Venezuela
and Paraguay simply refuse to feed the children of their wives if the children
are from another union, or simply demand that the children be put to death.
Few conclusions can be derived from these tendencies. Certainly, in
humans, the tendency to murder stepchildren can not be determined as purely
genetic. One could say that the cause is primarily social, as the stepchildren
are from broken families where there is likely more tension and parental
hostility towards children. Neither can animals? desire to kill the offspring of
their mate that are not their genetic children be explained. Whether the desire
to kill non-biological offspring is based on biology, sociology, or simple
emotion, this example displays the difficulty of pinning any sort of aggressive
or criminal behavior to a gene. It is also an example of the difficulty of using
social and genetic evidence, together, to track the source of any animal
behavior.
Society and Genetics
In the ten leading causes of death, violence kills more children than
disease. In 1988, 8150 US children between the ages if one and fourteen; 840 of
the deaths were clearly determined to be homicide; 237 were suicide. Homicide is
the fourth leading cause of death for children between one and nine years old,
and in the fifteen to twenty-four age group, it is the second leading cause of
death. Obviously, crime and violence do a considerable amount of damage to many
American lives. Consequently, limited amounts of genetic and other biological
research is being performed in order to find a genetic link, if any, to
aggression resulting in violence and crime. In 1989, $20 million in funds were
dedicated to violence research; 5% of those funds were allocated to the biology
of violence. There is so much conflict over the use of funds dealing with the
genetics of violence that the National Institutes of Health (NIH) funds no
specific studies that attempt to link genes and violence.
In August of 1992, the NIH allocated $78,000 to fund a controversial
conference in an effort to assess the social implications of the Human Genome
Project. The support was immediately withdrawn after black political leaders and
psychologists charged the conference?s agenda as being racist. The main
opposition to the conference was formed by the Black Caucus, who argued that the
roots of crime are based on social causes, such as poverty, racism, and
unemployment, and these call on social solutions, not biological ones.
Finally, in September of 1995, some 70 biologists, criminologists,
historians, and philosophers gathered at a remote conference center in the
Chesapeake Bay region. It was an NIH-sponsored conference that had been
carefully planned for over three years, made possible with a $133,000 from the
NIH. Some of the scientists contended that if genes mold physiology, then they
must mold psychology, and thus, antisocial behavior including violent crime must
have a genetic component. Others at the conference pressed that evidence for
genetic linkage to crime is circumstantial and a “racist pseudoscience”.
Behind the tensions that seemed to dominate the conference was the
horrors of past eugenics: the early twentieth-century campaign in the United
States, and later in Germany, to purify the human gene pool by sterilizing the
“feeble-minded.” The leaders of the eugenics movement in the United States,
although they acted out of sincere desire to build a better society, could do
little when their ideas took root in Nazi Germany in the 1930?s and soon became
the Holocaust; this is where much genetic tension and fear stem from. One of
the researchers, David Wasserman, a soft-spoken legal scholar, was shouting at
the top of his lungs that, “There are a hell of a lot of people attending this
conference who think the dangers of genetic research are as great in the long
term as the dangers of atomic energy!” Many critics argued that the genetic
studies are worse than inconclusive; they are racist and dangerous as they
generally fail to recognize social issues. William Schneider, an Indiana
University historian, in a formal protest statement, wrote, “Scientists as well
as historians and sociologists must not allow themselves to be used to provide
academic responsibility for racist pseudoscience.”
Flag-waving demonstrators, including self described communists, members
of the Progressive Labor Party, and representatives of Support Coalition
International (an alliance of psychiatric survivors endorsing a program against
psychiatric medication) stormed the auditorium and seized the microphones. A
student from Rutgers University proclaimed that, “You might think that you have
the right to do the research that you are doing, but the bottom line is that it
will be used to subjugate people.” It took two hours to clear out the
protesters and another eight hours to bring the proceedings to a close. A few
researchers admitted that they needed an eye-opener to see the social
implications of behavioral genetics dealing with violence and crime, realizing
that “Only historians have never had their results misused.”
Other federal research agencies have proposed a variety of monetary
packages to promote this research, and it is estimated that these funded
projects will cost the taxpayers as much as $50 million. However, this is not
the main concern of the opponents to this research. It is assumed that very
little is, at present, known about the human mind and its tendencies. Many
believe that there is an over-reliance on drugs therapy in psychiatry, and that
genetic violence research is cloaking the real problem. For example,
overwhelming numbers of black children with problems with violence and
aggressiveness are sent to psychiatrists where they are prescribed to pacifying
drugs such as Ritalin of Prozac. Many black leaders felt that it is impossible
to believe that the genetic studies are not attempting to find a link between
violence and race. The conference, while ultimately displaying the public?s fear
of genetic assessment and engineering, made little headway in determining the
course of the future of genetic research with regard to crime. It was, however,
a critical step in beginning to assess the risks and concerns, along with the
positive aspects, of behavioral genetics.
Conclusion
Genetic research and engineering, like any other new technology, has to
be carefully put to use, and in the right hands. It seems impossible to dismiss
any genetic research dealing with violence simply because it is has the
possibility to become dangerous and fall into the wrong hands. Like nuclear
research, genetics can be used for many positive deeds and the advancement of
man. While I think that genetic research dealing with violence and genetics
could have many positive aspects, it seems necessary to perform genetic research
on all varieties of people: criminals, white-collar businessmen, the white-house
staff and used car salesmen. Criminals cannot be singled out as the group that
needs “healing”; genetic research can ultimately benefit all people, therefore,
it must be performed on a variety of people. I, like many others, with the
widespread use of psychotherapeutic drugs, such as Prozac and Ritalin, fear and
foresee a day when designer drugs are used by all in order to help them deal
with society. This is, personally, the most frightening possibility resulting
from behavioral genetic research.
A time will never come when all are avid proponents of genetic
engineering for the betterment of society. People need to decide for themselves
whether research should continue, and to what degree. In the end, it will be the
common people who will decide the course of genetic research, not the scientists.
And, in the event of genetic developments, it should not only be the personal
decision of the individual as to how they will personally use the new
development, but the individual?s responsibility to design a solid opinion of
their moral, ethical, and biological feelings regarding the employment of
behavioral genetics in the future.
Bibliography
Brunner, H. G., et al., Abnormal Behavior Associated with a Point Mutation in
the Structural Gene for Monoamine Oxidase A, Science, Vol. 161, 22
October 1993.
Goldberg, Jeff, The Bad Seed: Amid Controversy, Scientists Hunt for the
“Aggression” Gene, Omni, Vol.17, Iss. 5, February 1995.
Hilts, Philip J., Evolutionists Take the Long View on Sex and Violence, Science,
Vol 261, 20 August 1993.
Holden, Constance, NIH Kills Genes and Crime Grant, Science, Vol 260, Iss. 5108,
30 April, 1993.
McBeath, Michael K., Genetic Hint to Schizophrenia, Nature, Vol 340, No. 6321,
May 13, 1995.
Oberbye, Dennis, Born to Raise Hell, Time, Vol. 143, Iss. 8, 21 February, 1994.
Palca, Joseph, NIH Wrestles with Furor over Conference, Science, Vol. 257, Iss.
5071, 7 August, 1992.
Richardson, Sara, Violence in the Blood, Discover, Vol. 355, No. 4553, October
1993.
Roush, Wade, Conflict Marks Crime Conference, Science, Vol. 269, Iss. 5232, 29
September, 1995
Stone, Richard, HHS ?Violence and Initiative? Caught in a Crossfire, Science,
Vol. 258, Iss. 5080, 9 October, 1992.
Stephens, Jane Ellen, The Biology of Violence, Bioscience, Vol. 44, Iss. 5, May
1994.
! |
Как писать рефераты Практические рекомендации по написанию студенческих рефератов. |
! | План реферата Краткий список разделов, отражающий структура и порядок работы над будующим рефератом. |
! | Введение реферата Вводная часть работы, в которой отражается цель и обозначается список задач. |
! | Заключение реферата В заключении подводятся итоги, описывается была ли достигнута поставленная цель, каковы результаты. |
! | Оформление рефератов Методические рекомендации по грамотному оформлению работы по ГОСТ. |
→ | Виды рефератов Какими бывают рефераты по своему назначению и структуре. |
Реферат | Christmas Carol Essay Research Paper |
Реферат | Sex Now And Then Essay Research |
Реферат | Экономические игры |
Реферат | Зміни в системі місцевого самоврядування, внесені реформою Катерини II |
Реферат | Петушок и его семья |
Реферат | Джон Ланкастерский, герцог Бедфорд |
Реферат | The New Deal Essay Research Paper |
Реферат | Tragedy Of King Lear Essay Research Paper |
Реферат | Соціально-правовий захист дітей від насильства в сім ї |
Реферат | A Christmas Carol Essay Research Paper Charles |
Реферат | LORD OF THE FLIES The Stronger |
Реферат | The Great Gatsby Nick Versus Gatsby Essay |
Реферат | The New Deal 2 |
Реферат | Обучение детей письму укр |
Реферат | Устройство родного языка |