Реферат по предмету "На английском языке"


False Memory Essay Research Paper Memory is

False Memory Essay, Research Paper
Memory is the mental faculty of retaining and recalling past experiences. A
repressed memory is one that is retained in the sub conscious mind, where
one is not aware of it but where it can still affect both conscious thoughts
and behavior. When memory is distorted or confabulated, the result can be what has been
called the False Memory Syndrome: a condition in which a person’s identity
and interpersonal relationships are entered around a memory of traumatic
experience which is objectively false but in which the person strongly
believes. Note that the syndrome is not characterized by false memories as
such. We all have memories that are inaccurate. Rather, the syndrome may be
diagnosed when the memory is so deeply ingrained that it orients the
individual’s entire personality and lifestyle, in turn disrupting all sorts
of other adaptive behaviors. The analogy to personality disorder is
intentional. False memory syndrome is especially destructive because the
person assiduously avoids confrontation with any evidence that might
challenge the memory. Thus it takes on a life of its own, encapsulated and
resistant to correction. The person may become so focused on the memory that
he or she may be effectively distracted from coping with real problems in
his or her life. — John F. Kihlstrom, Ph.D. There are many models which try to explain how memory works. Nevertheless,
we do not know exactly how memory works. One of the most questionable models
of memory is the one which assumes that every experience a person has had is
‘recorded’ in memory and that some of these memories are of traumatic events
too terrible to want to remember. These terrible memories are locked away in
the sub conscious mind, i.e. repressed, only to be remembered in adulthood
when some triggering event opens the door to the unconscious. And, both
before and after the repressed memory is remembered, it causes physical and
mental disorders in a person. Some people have made an effort to explain their pain, even cancer, as
coming from repressed memories of incest in the body. Scientists have
studied related phenomenon such as people whose hands bleed in certain
religious settings. Presumably such people, called stigmatics, “are not
revealing unconscious memories of being crucified as young children, but
rather are demonstrating a fascinating psychogenic anomaly that springs from
their conscious fixation on the suffering of Christ. Similarly, it is
possible that conscious fixation on the idea that one was sexually abused
might increase the frequency of some physical symptoms, regardless of
whether or not the abuse really occurred.”(Lindsay & Read, 1994) This view of memory has two elements: (1) the accuracy element and (2) the
causal element. The reason this model is questionable is not because people
don’t have unpleasant or painful experiences they would rather forget, nor
is it claiming that children often experience both wonderful and brutal
things for which they have no conceptual or linguistic framework and hence
are incapable of understanding them, much less relating it to others. It is
questionable because this model maintains that because (a) one is having
problems of functioning as a healthy human being and (b) one remembers being
abused as a child that therefore (A) one was abused as a child and (B) the
childhood abuse is the cause of one’s adulthood problems. There is no evidence that supports the claim that we remember everything
that we experience. In fact, there is plenty of evidence to support the
claim that it is impossible for us to even attend to all the perceptual
elements of any given experience, much less to recall them all. There is no
evidence to support the claim that all memories of experiences happened as
they remembered to have happened or that they have even happened at all. And
there is no evidence to support the claim that subjective certainty about
the accuracy of memories or the vividness of memories significantly
correlates with accuracy. Finally, the claim of a causal connection between
abuse and health or behavior does not warrant concluding that ill health,
mental or physical, is a ’sign’ of having been abused. This model is the basis for a number of pseudoscientific works on child
abuse by self-proclaimed experts such as Ellen Bass, E. Sue Blum, Laura
Davis, Beverly Engel, Beverly Holman, Wendy Maltz and Mary Jane Williams.
Through communal reinforcement many empirically unsupported notions,
including the claim that about half of all women have been sexually abused,
get treated as a ‘fact’ by many people. Psychologist Carol Tavris writes In what can only be called an incestuous arrangement, the authors of these
books all rely on one another’s work as supporting evidence for their own;
they all recommend one another’s books to their readers. If one of them
comes up with a concocted statistic–such as “more than half of all women
survivors of childhood sexual trauma “– the numbers are traded like
baseball cards, reprinted in every book and eventually enshrined as fact.
Thus the cycle of misinformation, faulty statistics and unvalidated
assertions maintains itself. (Tavris, 1993) The only difference between this group of experts and say, a group of
physicists is that the child abuse experts have achieved their status as
authorities not by scientific training but by either (a) experience [they
were victims themselves or they have treated victims of abuse in their
capacity as social workers] or (b) they wrote a book on child abuse. The
child abuse experts are not trained in scientific research which is not a
comment on their ability to write or to do therapy, but which does seem to
be one reason for their scientific illiteracy. (Tavris, 1993) Here are a few of the unproved, unscientifically researched notions that
are being bandied around by these child abuse experts: One, if you doubt
that you were abused as a child or think that it might be your imagination,
this is a sign of ‘post-incest syndrome’. Two, if you can not remember any
specific instances of being abused, but still have a feeling that something
abusive happened to you, ‘it probably did’. Three, when a person can not
remember his or her childhood or have very fuzzy memories, ‘incest must
always be considered as a possibility’. And four, ‘If you have any suspicion
at all, if you have any memory, no matter how vague, it probably really
happened. It is far more likely that you are blocking the memories, denying
it happened’.
There have been many symptoms suggested as indicators of past abuse. These
symptoms range from headaches to irritable bowls. In fact, one psychologist
compiled a list of over 900 different symptoms that had been presented as
proof of a history of abuse. When he reviewed the professional literature,
he found that not one of the symptoms could be shown to be an inclusive
indication of a history of abuse. Given the lack of consistent scientific
evidence, therapists must be careful in declaring that abuse has infact
occurred. (London, 1995) Whole industries have been built up out of the hysteria that inevitably
accompanies charges of the sexual abuse of children. Therapists who are
supposed to help children recover from the trauma of the abuse are hired to
interrogate the child, in order to find out if they have been abused. But
all too often the therapist suggests the abuse to the child and the child
has ‘memories’ of being abused, but no rational person should find a parent
or caretaker guilty on the basis of such tainted testimony. [note 1] Increasingly throughout the continent, grown children under going
therapeutic programs have come to believe that they suffer from “repressed
memories” of incest and sexual abuse. While some reports of incest and
sexual abuse are surely true, these decade delayed memories are too often
the result of False Memory Syndrome caused by a disastrous “therapeutic”
program. False Memory Syndrome has a devastating effect on the victim and
typically produces a continuing dependency on the very program that creates
the syndrome. False Memory Syndrome proceeds to destroy the psychological
well being not only of the primary victim but through false accusations of
incest and sexual abuse other members of the primary victim’s family. The
American Medical Association considers recovered memories of childhood
sexual abuse to be of uncertain authenticity, which should be subject to
external verification. The use of recovered memories is fraught with
problems of potential misapplication.[note 2] The dangers of this model are apparent: not only are false memories treated
as real memories, but real memories of real abuse may be treated as false
memories and may provide real abusers with a believable defense. In the end,
no one benefits from encouraging a belief in memory which is unfounded.
Whatever the theory of memory one advocates, if it does not entail examining
corroborating evidence and attempting to independently verify claims of
recollected abuse, it is a theory which will cause more harm than good. Carl Jung, an early Freudian disciple and later heretic, extended this
model of memory by adding another area of repressed memories to the
unconscious mind, an area that was not based on individual past experiences
at all: the “collective” unconscious. The collective unconscious is the
repository for acts and mental patterns shared either by members of a
culture or universally by all humans. Under certain conditions these
manifest themselves as archetype: images, patterns and symbols, that are
often seen in dreams or fantasies and that appear as themes in mythology,
religion and fairy tales. The Archetype of the Archetype Model can be traced
back to Plato’s various beliefs about the eidos. (Forms of reality which
were variously described by Plato but always were held up as ‘more real’
than the world of sense experience which, in some way, was always held up as
inferior to and dependant on the eidos.) The Platonic Model avoids the problem of determining whether or not a
memory is accurate by claiming that the memory is not of a personal
experience at all. It also confuses several types of mental states. It
completely blurs the distinction between dream states and conscious states
by eliminating the difference between remembering a sense experience one
actually had and remembering a sense experience one never actually had. This
model gives validity to every fantasy and desire. If one is clever, though,
one can destroy the first model with the second one. For example, a Jungian
could claim that the repressed memories of all those who are now blaming
their current troubles on forgotten and repressed memories of child abuse,
are not memories of actual abuse but of an Archetype, the Abused Child
Archetype. The story of Hansel and Gretel might be pulled in for
“scientific” support of the idea. Unsupported assertions might be made
regarding the unconscious desire of all children to be loved by their
parents: as children, love could only be understood in terms of ego
gratification, but as adults love is understood primarily in sexual terms.
Because of the incest taboo, we can not bear the thought of wanting to be
loved sexually by our parents, so this desire must be expressed in a
perverse and inverse way: our parents love us sexually. But there is no
evidence for this based upon our past or current relationship with our
parents, so the mind creates the evidence by remembering being sexually
abused as a child. Thus, the memory we have as adults of being sexually
abused by our parents is actually the expression of the universal desire to
be loved by our mother and father. It has nothing to do with any real
experience; it has everything to do with a universal human desire. It also
serves as a convenient excuse to absolve us of all responsibility for our
failures and incompetence. The reason we are so screwed up is because our
parents screwed us! How accurate and reliable is memory? We’re often wrong in thinking we
accurately remember things. Studies on memory have shown that we often
construct our memories after the fact, that we are susceptible to
suggestions from others that help us fill in the gaps in our memories of
certain events. (Hyman, Jr., Husband & Billings, 1995) That is why, for
example, a police officer investigating a crime should not show a picture of
a single individual to a victim and ask if the victim recognizes the
assailant. If the victim is then presented a line up and picks out the
individual whose picture the victim had been shown, there is no way of
knowing whether the victim is remembering the assailant or the picture. Another interesting fact about memory is that studies have shown that there
is no significant correlation between the subjective feeling of certainty a
person has about memory and that memory being accurate. Also, contrary to
what many believe, hypnosis does not aid memory’s accuracy because subjects
are extremely suggestible while under hypnosis. (Loftus, 1980) It is
possible to create false memories in people’s minds by suggestion. The mind does not record every detail of an event, but only a few features;
we fill in the rest on what “must have been.” For an event to make it to
long term storage, a person has to perceive it, encode it and rehearse it
–tell about it– or it decays. (This seems to be the major mechanism behind
childhood amnesia, the fact that children do not develop long term memory
until roughly age three.) Otherwise, research finds, even emotional
experiences we are sure we will never forget –the Kennedy assassination,
the Challenger explosion– will fade from memory, and errors will creep into
the account that remain.(Travis, 1993) Research articles and court testimony confirm the wide spread use of memory
enhancement techniques, in the belief that these will help recover accurate
“memories”. These techniques include hypnosis, sodium amytal, dream
interpretation, guided imagery, journaling, body massages, participation in
survivor groups and reading of self help books. In the summer of 1993, the
American Medical Association passed a resolution warning of the dangers of
misapplication in the use of these techniques. In June of 1994 they issued a
warning about all recovered memories. Both the AMA and the American
Psychiatric Association have stated: ..there is no completely accurate way of determining the validity of reports
in the absence of corroborating information.[note 3] The problem with the practices mentioned above is that when they are used
they increase the risk of influence and suggestibility. Why would someone remember something so horrible if it really did not
happen? This is a haunting question, but there are several possible
explanations which might shed light on some of the false memories. A
pseudomemory, for example, may be a kind of symbolic expression of troubled
family relationships. There may be a cultural climate in our society in
which the belief in the relationship between sexual abuse and individual
pathology is nurtured. It may be that in such a climate people more readily
believe things happened when they didn’t. When people enter therapy, they do
so to get better. They want to change. People also tend to look for some
explanation for why they have a problem. Clients come to trust the person
they have chosen to help them. Because they are trying to get better,
clients tend to rely on the therapist’s opinion. If the therapist believes
that the reason that the client has a problem is because of some past
trauma, and especially if the therapist believes that the patient will not
get better unless he or she remembers the trauma, the patient will work to
find what he or she thinks is a trauma memory in order to get better.
Richard Ofshe, Ph.D. and Ethan Watters noted that, “No one — not the
patients, therapists, parents or critics of recovered memory therapy –
question that this therapy is an intensely difficult and painful experience.
That the pain of therapy is real should not be accepted, however, as an
argument that the memories uncovered are accurate. One’s emotional reaction
to a perceived memory need not correlate with the veracity of that event,
but rather only to whether one believes that event to be true.”[note 4] Therapists may believe that they are helping clients and improving a
culture in which sex abuse is far too prevalent. A patient may find group
acceptance in the cadre of survivors and find “the” reason for problems.
Patients suffering from severe psychological symptoms are known to engage in
what is called, “effort after meaning” (Bass & Davis, 1988), in that they
seek some explanation, however remote, for suffering. So, should accounts of repressed memory be dismissed out of hand? Of course
not! But there should be an attempt to corroborate such memories with
independent evidence and testimony before drawing conclusions about actual
abuses or crimes. Such accounts should be taken very seriously and should be
critically examined, giving them all the attention and investigative
analysis we would give to any allegation of crime. But we should not rush to
judgement, either about the accuracy of the memories of about the causal
connection between past experiences and present problems. We should neither
automatically reject as false memories which have been repressed for years
and are suddenly recollected, nor should we automatically accept such
memories as true. In terms of verification of their accuracy, these memories
should not be treated any differently than any other type of memory. NOTES 1. Yet, it has happened. In a modern version of the Salem witch hunts, the
McMartin pre-school case exemplifies the very worst in institutionalized
justice on the hunt for child molesters. See, Mason, M. (Sept. 1991). The McMartin case revisited: the conflict
between social work and criminal justice, Social Work, v.36, no.5.
391-396. [on evaluating the credibility of children as witnesses in sexual
abuse cases], 2. See, Council on Scientific Affairs, (1994). American Medical
Association, June 16. 3. See, Council on Scientific Affairs, (1994). American Medical
Association, June 16. 4. See, Ofshe, R., & Watters, E., (1994). Making Monsters: False Memory,
Psychotherapy and Sexual Hysteria. p.109. REFERENCES Bass, E. & Davis, L., (1988). The Courage To Heal, p.173. Council on Scientific Affairs, (1994). American Medical Association, June 16. Hyman, I.E. Jr., Husband, T.H. & Billings, F.J., (1995). Prompting false
childhood memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, pp.181-197. Lindsay, S. & Read, D., (1994). Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, p.302. London., (1995). Independent Practitioner, March 1, 64. Loftus, E., (1980). Memory, Surprising New Insights Into How We Remember
and Why We Forget, Reading, Mass,: Addison-Wesley Pub. Co. Loftus, E., & Ketcham, K., (1987). Eye Witness Testimony: Civil and
Criminal, New York, N.Y.: Kluwer Law Book Publishers. Loftus, E., (1980). Eye Witness Testimony, Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard
University Press. Mason, M., (Sept. 1991). The McMartin case revisited: the conflict between
social work and criminal justice,” Social Work, 36, no. 5, pp.391-396. Ofshe, R., & Watters, E., (1994). Making Monsters: False Memory,
Psychotherapy and Sexual Hysteria. p.109. Tavris, C., (1993). Hysteria and the Incest Survivor Machine, Sacramento
Bee, Forum section, January 17, p.1.


Не сдавайте скачаную работу преподавателю!
Данный реферат Вы можете использовать для подготовки курсовых проектов.

Поделись с друзьями, за репост + 100 мильонов к студенческой карме :

Пишем реферат самостоятельно:
! Как писать рефераты
Практические рекомендации по написанию студенческих рефератов.
! План реферата Краткий список разделов, отражающий структура и порядок работы над будующим рефератом.
! Введение реферата Вводная часть работы, в которой отражается цель и обозначается список задач.
! Заключение реферата В заключении подводятся итоги, описывается была ли достигнута поставленная цель, каковы результаты.
! Оформление рефератов Методические рекомендации по грамотному оформлению работы по ГОСТ.

Читайте также:
Виды рефератов Какими бывают рефераты по своему назначению и структуре.